In the past two decades, researchers have made great strides in uncovering how children learn maths, but little of that new knowledge has trickled down to teachers, according to a new book on maths education.
The gap between research and practice is particularly unfortunate, given the current state of American students’ maths skills, said Nancy Krasa, co-author of How Children Learn Math: The Science of Math Learning in Research and Practice.
“American children are not doing well at maths,” said Krasa, who is an adjunct assistant professor of psychology at The Ohio State University.
“In 2019, only about one-fourth of high school seniors scored at or above the proficiency level in maths. And all indications are that this has only gotten worse with the learning loss associated with the COVID-19 pandemic.”
But there is a way to meet this challenge, according to Krasa, who is a licensed psychologist specialising in children with learning difficulties, including those who have trouble with maths.
“The scientific research on how children learn maths has exploded in the past 20 years, with thousands of new studies focused on how children come to understand numbers and various other aspects of math,” she said.
“The problem is that little of this work has been accessible to teachers on the front line.”
Krasa said she and her co-authors, Karen Tzanetopoulos and Colleen Maas, wrote How Children Learn Math to bring the latest discoveries in maths learning to teachers and parents and give them research-based ways to teach the fundamentals to young students. The book focuses on toddlerhood through the learning of fractions.
A good example of one of the new discoveries is learning spatial skills. The importance of spatial skills in early maths is one of those crucial findings that hasn’t made it to early education teachers, Krasa said.
Most people think of spatial skills in terms of geometry, but recent research suggests that a person’s spatial skills are associated with their maths skills more generally.
“That’s something most teachers would have no idea about, but the results are remarkably consistent,” she said.
“What is not yet entirely clear is how they are related – why do people with good spatial skills have an easier time with maths?”
One hypothesis is that humans think of numerical quantities along a mental number line as if they existed in space. One real-life classroom application is that a physical number line in the classroom, if properly used, may help teach young children about numbers.
But research shows that children begin developing spatial skills even before they get to school. One way very young children learn spatial skills is by playing with blocks.
One study Krasa and her co-authors mention in the book found that when mothers and their 3-year-olds build with blocks together, the amount of spatial language, related gestures, and planning support the mothers provide predicts the children’s maths skills in first grade.
The impact of playing with blocks and its effects on spatial skills goes well beyond the early grades.
Another study found that children’s preschool block-building skills predicted their high school maths course selections, maths grades and standardised maths scores.
One implication of recent research is that children should be screened for spatial skills early in life, just as they are for reading skills, Krasa said. The good news is that “spatial skills are trainable, especially if we can identify those who need help early.”
Another important finding of recent research is the importance of language in learning maths, she said.
“Maths language is very abstract. Students may understand maths concepts better with familiar terms, such as ‘and’ instead of ‘plus’, for example.”
“Also, maths is not separate from reading. Research has shown that children with reading disabilities, particularly dyslexia, are at a great risk for maths failure.”
One study found that of children who had been diagnosed with a developmental language disorder in kindergarten, 55% had serious maths difficulties by the fifth grade – more than 10 times the rate found in the general population.
Despite the alarming statistics about maths knowledge among American children, Krasa said working on the book has convinced her that the situation is not hopeless.
“I believe that with the proper support, all children in the normal range of intelligence can learn maths. Even with challenges like poverty, reading and language disability, weak spatial skills and attentional issues, they can learn and understand the fundamental concepts,” she said.
The key is that students have to begin early, or, if they don’t, they have to go back and begin with the fundamentals. Maths skills and concepts that students learn in high school are built on those from elementary school – and those are built on skills learned in preschool and at home.
That means many of the problems that students face in high school find their roots in early maths education.
“If we’re going to get it right, we have to start from the beginning,” Krasa said.
The US failure in maths education is not the fault of teachers, she said. They are doing the best they can given their training and the challenges they face.
“We want teachers to have the latest research on how children actually learn maths so they can help turn things around. That’s why we wrote this book.”
The book offers activities that are easily understandable for teachers and parents, but that isn’t currently being used in most classrooms, she said.
These new approaches are desperately needed.
“Clearly, something is not working in maths education in this country. We could be doing much, much better,” Krasa said.